Social stress

Social stress is stress that stems from one’s relationships with others and from the social environment in general. A person experiences stress when he or she does not have the ability or resources to cope when confronted with an external stimulus (stressor), or when they fear they do not have the ability or resources. An event which exceeds the ability to cope does not necessarily have to occur in order for one to experience stress, as the threat of such an event occurring can be sufficient. This can lead to emotional, behavioral and physiological changes that can put one under greater risk for developing mental disorder and physical illness.

Humans are social beings by nature, as they typically have a fundamental need and desire to maintain positive social relationships [1]. Thus, they usually find maintaining positive social ties to be beneficial. In particular, social relationships can offer nurturance, foster feelings of social inclusion, and even lead to reproductive success [2]. As a result, anything that disrupts or threatens to disrupt their relationships with others can result in social stress. This can include low social status in society or in particular groups, giving a speech, interviewing with potential employers, caring for a child or spouse with a chronic illness, meeting new people at a party, the threat of or actual death of a loved one, divorce, and discrimination. These social stressors convey that social stress can arise from one’s micro-environment (e.g., family ties) and macro-environment (e.g., hierarchical societal structure). Given the social nature of humans, it is not surprising that social stress is typically the most frequent type of stressor that people experience in their daily lives and affects people more intensely than other types of stressors [3].

Contents

Measurement

Social stress is typically studied by asking people about their social experiences and relationships or by inducing social stress in the laboratory.

Self-reports

A variety of questionnaires are used to assess self-reports of stressful social experiences, including the Test of Negative Social Exchange (TENSE) [4], Martial Adjustment Test [5], and the Risky Families Questionnaire [6]. Richer, more detailed information can be gathered by asking open-ended questions about one’s recent social stress experiences. For instance, the UCLA Life Stress Interview includes questions about romantic partners, closest friendships, other friendships, and family relationships.

Induction

In rodent models, social disruption and social defeat are two common social stress paradigms. In the social disruption paradigm, an aggressive rodent is introduced into a cage housing male rodents that have already naturally established a social hierarchy. The aggressive "intruder" disrupts the social hierarchy, causing the residents social stress. In the social defeat paradigm, an aggressive "intruder" and another non-aggressive male rodent fight. Presumably, the non-aggressive rodent will be defeated, an outcome causing social stress.

In human research, the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST) and conflict discussion are common inductions of social stress. In the TSST, participants have to give an impromptu speech and then perform mental arithmetic as quickly and accurately as possible. Both tasks are completed in front of a judging panel that is trained to provide nonverbal negative feedback [7]. The threat of negative evaluation is the social stressor. In a laboratory conflict discussion between two people, a topic of disagreement is determined and the participants are asked to discuss the topic for a predetermined amount of time. Discussion of the topic will presumably lead to tension.

The variety of ways to assess social stress reflects the fact that social defeat, family environment, quality of relationships, social evaluation, and conflict are all specific social stressors. The decision to use a particular questionnaire or manipulation depends on the type of social stress one is interested in.

Mental Health

In line with the notion that humans have a fundamental need for social inclusion and maintenance of social relationships, research has consistently demonstrated that social stress increases risk for developing a number of negative mental health outcomes. One prospective study asked over fifteen hundred Finnish employees whether they had “considerable difficulties with [their] coworkers/superiors/inferiors during the last 6 months, 5 years, earlier, or never” [8]. Information on suicidal deaths, hospitalizations due to psychosis, suicidal behavior, alcohol intoxication, and depressive symptoms, and medication for chronic psychiatric disorders, was then gathered from the national registries of mortality and morbidity. Those who had experienced conflict in the workplace with coworkers and/or supervisors in the last five years were more likely to be diagnosed with a psychiatric condition [8].

Depression

Risk for developing depression significantly increases after experiencing social stress [9] and in fact, depressed individuals often experience interpersonal loss before becoming depressed [10-11]. One study found that depressed individuals who had been rejected by others had developed depression about three times more quickly than those who had experienced non-socially rejecting stress [12]. In non-clinically depressed populations, people with friends and family who make too many demands, criticize, and create tension and conflict tend to have more depressive symptoms [13-15]. Conflict between spouses also leads to more psychological distress and depressive symptoms, especially for wives [16]. In particular, unhappy married couples are 10-25 times more at risk for developing major depression [17-18]. Relatedly, social stress arising from discrimination is related to greater depressive symptoms. In one study, African-Americans and non-Hispanic Whites reported on their daily experiences of discrimination and depressive symptoms. Regardless of race, those who perceived more discrimination had higher depressive symptoms [19].

Long-term effects

Social stress occurring early in life can have psychopathological effects that develop or persist in adulthood. One longitudinal study found that children were more likely to have a psychiatric disorder (i.e., anxiety, depressive, disruptive, personality, and substance use disorder) in late adolescence and early adulthood when their parents showed more maladaptive child-rearing behaviors (i.e., loud arguments between parents, verbal abuse, difficulty controlling anger toward the child, parental support/availability, and harsh punishment). Child temperament and parental psychiatric disorders did not explain this association [20]. Other studies have documented the robust relationships between children’s social stress within the family environment and depression, aggression, antisocial behavior, anxiety, suicide, and hostile, oppositional, and delinquent behavior [21].

Relapse and recurrence

Social stress can also exacerbate current psychopathological conditions and compromise recovery. For instance, patients recovering from depression or bipolar disorder are two times more likely to relapse if there is familial tension. People with eating disorders are also more likely to relapse if their family members make more critical comments, are more hostile, or are over-involved [22]. Similarly, outpatients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder show greater psychotic symptoms if the most influential person in their life is critical [23] and are more likely to relapse if their familial relationships are marked by tension [22]. In regard to substance abuse, cocaine-dependent individuals report greater cravings for cocaine following exposure to a social stressor [24].

Physical Health

Research has also repeatedly found a robust relationship between various social stressors and aspects of physical health.

Mortality

Social status, a macro social stressor, is a robust predictor of death. In a study of over seventeen hundred British civil servants, socioeconomic status (SES) was related to mortality in a gradient fashion [25]. It is not just that those with the lowest SES have worse health than those with the greatest SES. Rather, with each step up the socioeconomic ladder, longer life is predicted. Other studies have replicated this relationship between SES and mortality in a range of diseases including infectious, digestive, and respiratory diseases [26-27]. Similarly, social stressors in the microenvironment are also linked to increased mortality. A seminal longitudinal study of nearly seven thousand people found that socially isolated people had greater risk of dying from all causes [28].

Morbidity

Social stress also makes people sicker. People who have fewer social contacts are at greater risk for developing illness, including cardiovascular disease [29]. The lower one’s social status, the more likely he or she is to have a cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, musculoskeletal, neoplastic, pulmonary, renal, or other chronic disease. These links are not explained by other, more traditional risk factors such as race, health behaviors, age, sex, and even access to health care [30]. Interpersonally, negative social interactions also lead to sickness. In one particularly noteworthy laboratory study, researchers interviewed participants to determine whether they had been experiencing social conflicts with spouses, close family members and friends. They then exposed the participants to the common cold virus and found that participants with conflict-ridden relationships were two times more likely to develop a cold than those without such social stress [31].

Long-term effects

Exposure to social stress in childhood can also have long-term effects, increasing risk for developing diseases later in life. In particular, adults who were maltreated (emotionally, physically, sexually abused or neglected) as children report more disease outcomes, such as stroke, myocardial infarction, diabetes, and hypertension [32] or greater severity of those outcomes [33]. The Childhood Experiences Study, which includes over seventeen hundred adults, also found that there was a 20% increase in likelihood for experiencing cardiovascular heart disease (CHD) for each kind of chronic familial social stressor experienced in childhood, and this was not due to typical risk factors for CHD such as demographics, smoking, exercise, adiposity, diabetes, and hypertension [34].

Recovery

Social stress has also been tied to worse health outcomes among patients who already have a disease. Patients with end-stage renal disease faced a 46% increased risk for mortality when there was more relationship negativity with their spouse even when controlling for severity of disease and treatment [35]. Similarly, women who had experienced an acute coronary event were three times more likely to experience another coronary event if they experienced moderate to severe marital strain. This finding remained even after controlling for demographics, health behaviors, and disease status [36].

Physiological Pathways

Social stress leads to a number of physiological changes that mediate its relationship to physical health. In the short term, the physiological changes outlined below are adaptive, as they enable the stressed organism to cope better. However, dysregulation of these systems or repeated activation of them over the long-term can be detrimental to health [37].

Sympathetic adrenomedullary system (SAM)

The sympathetic adrenomedullary system becomes activated in response to stress. Sympathetic arousal stimulates the medulla of the adrenal glands to secrete epinephrine and norepinephrine into the blood stream, which facilitates the fight or flight response. Blood pressure, heart rate, and sweating increase, veins constrict to allow the heart to beat with more force, arteries leading to muscles dilate, and blood flow to parts of the body not essential for the fight or flight response decreases. If stress persists in the long run, then blood pressure remains elevated, leading to hypertension and atherosclerosis, both precursors to cardiovascular disease.

A number of animal and human studies have confirmed that social stress increases risk for negative health outcomes by increasing SAM activity. Studies of rodents show that social stress causes hypertension and atherosclerosis [38]. Studies of non-human primates also show that social stress clogs arteries [39]. Although humans cannot be randomized to receive social stress due to ethical concerns, studies have nevertheless shown that negative social interactions characterized by conflict lead to increases in blood pressure and heart rate [40-41]. Social stress stemming from perceived daily discrimination is also associated with elevated levels of blood pressure during the day and a lack of blood pressure dipping at night [42-43].

Hypothalamic-pituitary adrenocortical axis (HPA)

The HPA axis also gets activated in response to stress. The hypothalamus releases corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH), stimulating the pituitary to release adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH). ACTH then stimulates the adrenal cortex to secrete glucocorticoids, including cortisol. The levels of cortisol in the bloodstream then signal to the hypothalamus to stop secreting CRH so as to stop the cascade of events leading to increased production of cortisol.

It is thought that social stress can lead to adverse health outcomes by chronically and/or repeatedly activating the HPA axis or disrupting the HPA system, and there are a number of studies that link social stress and indications of a disrupted HPA axis. For instance, monkey infants neglected by their mothers show prolonged cortisol responses following a challenging event [44]. In humans, abused women exhibit a prolonged elevation in cortisol following a standardized psychosocial laboratory stressor compared to those without an abuse history [45]. Maltreated children not only show higher morning cortisol values than non maltreated children [46], but their HPA systems also fail to recover after a stressful social interaction with their caregiver [47]. Over time, low-SES children show progressively greater output of cortisol [48-49]. Although these studies point to a disrupted HPA system accounting for the link between social stress and physical health, they did not include disease outcomes. Nevertheless, a dysfunctional HPA response to stress is thought to increase risk for developing or exacerbating diseases such as diabetes, cancer, cardiovascular disease, and hypertension [50].

Inflammation

Inflammation is an immune response that is critical to fighting infections and repairing injured tissue. Although acute inflammation is adaptive, chronic inflammatory activity can contribute to adverse health outcomes, such as hypertension [51], atherosclerosis [52], coronary heart disease [53-54], depression [55], diabetes [56] and some cancers [57-58].

Research has elucidated a relationship between different social stressors and markers of inflammation, i.e., cytokines. Chronic social stressors such as caring for a spouse with dementia leads to greater circulating levels of cytokine interleukin-6 (IL-6) [59] whereas acute social stress tasks in the laboratory have been shown to elicit increases in pro-inflammatory cytokines [60]. Similarly, when faced with another type of social stress, namely social evaluative threat, participants showed increases in IL-6 and a soluble receptor for tumor necrosis factor-α [61-63]. Increases in inflammation may persist over time, as studies have shown that chronic relationship stress has been tied to greater IL-6 production 6 months later [64] and children reared in a stressful family environment marked by neglect and conflict tend to show elevated levels of C-reactive protein, a marker of IL-6, in adulthood [65].

Interactions of physiological systems

There is extensive evidence that the above physiological systems affect one another’s functioning. For instance, cortisol tends to have a suppressive effect on inflammatory processes and proinflammatory cytokines can also activate the HPA system [66]. Sympathetic activity can also upregulate inflammatory activity [67-68]. Given the relationships among these physiological systems, social stress may also influence health indirectly via affecting a particular physiological system that in turn affects a different physiological system.

See also

References

  1. Slavich, G.M., O’Donovan, A., Epel, E.S., & Kemeny, M.E. (2010). Black sheep get the blues: A psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 39-45.
  2. Baumeister, R.F., & Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
  3. Almeida, D.M. (2005). Resilience and vulnerability to daily stressors assessed via diary methods. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 64-68.
  4. Ruehlman, L.S., & Karoly, P. (1991). With a little flak from my friends: Development and preliminary validation of the test of negative social exchange (TENSE). Psychological Assessment, 3, 97-104.
  5. Locke, H.J. & Wallace, K.M. (1959). Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251-255.
  6. Taylor, S.E., Lerner, J.S., Sage, R.M., Lehman, B. J., & Seeman, T. E. (2004). Early environment, emotions, responses to stress, and health. Journal of Personality, 72, 1365-1393.
  7. Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.M., Hellhammer, D.H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiolog, 28, 76-81.
  8. Romanov, K., Appelberg, K., Honkasalo, M.L., & Koskenvuo, M. (1996). Recent interpersonal conflict at work and psychiatric morbidity: A prospective study of 15,530 employees aged 24-64. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40(2), 169-176.
  9. Monroe, S.M., Slavich, G.M., & Georgiades, K., 2009. The social environment and life stress in depression. In: Gotlib, L.H., Hammen, C.L. (Eds.), Handbook of Depression. Second ed. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 340-360.
  10. Paykel, E.S. (2003). Life events and affective disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 61– 66.
  11. Mazure, C.M. (1998). Life stressors as risk factors in depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 291-313.
  12. Slavich, G.M., Thornton, T., Torres, L.D., Monroe, S.M., & Gotlib, I.H., 2009. Targeted rejection predicts hastened onset of major depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 28, 223–243.
  13. Schuster, T.L., Kessler, R.C., & Aseltine, R.H. (1990). Supportive interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 423-438.
  14. Finch, J.F., Okun, M.A., Pool, G.J., & Ruehlman, L.S. (1999). A comparison of the influence of conflictual and supportive social interactions on psychological distress. Journal of Personality, 67(4), 581-621.
  15. Pinquart, M., & Sorensen, S. (2003). Associations of stressors and uplifts of caregiving with caregiver burden and depressive mood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological and Social Sciences, 58b(2), 112-128.
  16. Horwitz, A.V., McLaughlin, J., & White, H.R. (1997). How the negative and positive aspects of partner relationships affect the mental health of young married people. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 39, 124-136.
  17. Weissman, M.M. (1987). Advances in psychiatric epidemiology: Rates and risks for major depression. American Journal of Public Health, 77, 445–451.
  18. O’Leary, K.D., Christian, J.L., & Mendell, N.R. (1994). A closer look at the link between marital discord and depressive symptomatology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 33–41.
  19. Taylor, J. & Turner, R.J. (2002). Perceived discrimination, social strss, and depression in the transition to adulthood: Racial contrasts. Social Psychology Quarterly 65, (3), 213-225.
  20. Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., Smailes, E., & Brook, J. S. (2001). Association of maladaptive parental behavior with psychiatric disorder among parents and their offspring. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 453– 460.
  21. Repetti, R.L., Taylor, S.E., & Seeman, T.E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 330-366.
  22. Butzlaff, R.L. & Hooley, J.M. (1998). Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse: A meta- analyses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 547-552.
  23. Docherty, N.M., St-Hilaire, A., Aakre, J.M., Seghers, J.P., McCleery, A., & Divilbiss, M. (2011). Anxiety interacts with expressed emotion criticism in the prediction of psychotic symptom exacerbation. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(3), 611-618.
  24. Back, S.E., Hartwell, K., DeSantis, S.M., Saladin, M., McRae-Clark, A.L., Price, K.L., Moran-Santa Maria, M.M., Baker, N.L., Spratt, E., Kreek, M.J., & Brady, K.T. (2010). Reactivity to laboratory stress provocation predicts relapse to cocaine. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 106(1), 21-27.
  25. Marmot, M.G., Rose, G., Shipley, M., & Hamilton, P.J. (1978). Employment grade and coronary heart disease in British civil servants. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 32(4), 244-249.
  26. Adler, N.E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M.A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R.L., & Syme, S.L. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient.
  27. Kaplan, G.A. & Kell, J.E. (1993). Socioeconomic factors and cardiovascular disease: a review of the literature. Circulation, 88, 1973-1998.
  28. Berkman, L.F. & Syme, S.L. (1979) Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: A nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. American Journal of Epidemiology, 109, 186-204.
  29. Seeman, T.E. (2000). Health promoting effects of friends and family on health outcomes in older adults. American Journal of Health Promotion, 14(6), 362-370.
  30. Pincus, T., Callahan, L.F., & Burkhauser, R.V. (1987). Most chronic diseases are reported more frequently by individuals with fewer than 12 years of formal education in the age 18-64 U.S. population. Journal of Chronic Disease, 40, 865-874.
  31. Cohen, S., Frank, E., Doyle, W.J., Skoe, D.P., Rabin, B.S., & Gwaltney, J.M., Jr. (1998). Types of stressors that increase susceptibility to the common cold in healthy adults. Health Psychology, 17, 214-223.
  32. Rich-Edwards, J.W., Spiegelman, D., Lividoti Hibert, E.N., Jun, H., Todd, T.J., Kawachi, I., & Wright, R.J. (2010). Abuse in childhood and adolescence as a predictor of type 2 diabetes in adult women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 39(6), 529-536.
  33. Wegman, H.L. & Stetler, C. (2009). A meta-analytic review of the effects of childhood abuse on medical outcomes in adulthood. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 805-812.
  34. Dong, M., Giles, W.H., Felittie, V.J., Dube, S.R., Williams, J.E., Chapman, D.P., & Anda, R.F. (2004). Insight into causal pathways for ischemic heart disease: Adverse childhood experiences study. Circulation, 110, 1761-1766.
  35. Kimmel, P.L., Peterson, R.A., Weihs, K.L, Shidler, N., Simmens, S.J., Alleyne, S., Cruz, I., Yanovski, J.A., Veis, J.H., & Phillips, T.M. (2000). Dyadic relationship conflict, gender, and mortality in urban hemodialysis patients. Journal of the American Society of Nephrology, 11, 1518–1525.
  36. Orth-Gomer, K., Wamala, S.P., Horsten, M., Schenck-Gustafsson, K., Schneiderman, N., & Mittleman, M.A. (2000). Marital stress worsens prognosis in women with coronary heart disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 3008–3014.
  37. McEwen, B. S. & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the Individual: Mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153, 2093-2101.
  38. Sgoifo, A., Koolhaas, J., De Boer, S., Musso, E., Stilli, D., Buwalda, B., & Meerlo, P. (1999). Social stress, autonomic neural activation, and cardiac activity in rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 915-923.
  39. Manuck, S., Marsland, A., Kaplan, J., & Williams, J. (1995). The pathogenicity of behavior and its neuroendocrine mediation: An example from coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 275-283.
  40. Gerin, W., Pierper, C., Levy, R., & Pickering, T. (1992). Social support in social interaction: A moderator of cardiovascular reactivity. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 324-336.
  41. Nealey-Moore, J.B., Smith, T.W., Uchino, B.N., Hawkins, M.W., & Olson-Cerny, C. (2007). Cardiovascular reactivity during positive and negative marital interactions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30, 505-519.
  42. Tomfohr, L., Cooper, D.C., Mills, P.J., Nelesen, R.A., & Dimsdale, J.E. (2010). Everyday discrimination and nocturnal blood pressure dipping in black and white Americans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(3), 266-272.
  43. Richman, L.S., Pek, J., Pascoe, E., & Bauer, D.J. (2010), The effects of perceived discrimination on ambulatory blood pressure and affective responses to interpersonal stress modeled over 24 hours. Health Psychology 29(4), 403-411.
  44. Dettling, Al, Pryce, C.R., Martin, R.D., & Dobeli, M. (1998). Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goldi’s monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Developmental Psychobiology, 33(1), 21-31.
  45. Heim, C., Newport, D.J., Heit, S., Graham, .P., Wilcox, M., Bonsall, R., Miller, A.H., & Nemeroff, C.B. (2000). Increased pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in adult women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284
  46. Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001). The impact of child maltreatment and psychopathology upon neuroendocrine functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 783-804.
  47. Fries, A.B., Shirtcliff, E.A., & Pollak, S.D. (2008). Neuroendocrine dysregulation following early social deprivation in children. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(8), 588-599.
  48. Chen, E., Cohen, S., & Miller, G.E. (2010). How low soceioeconomic status affects 2-year hormonal trajectories in children. Psychological Science, 21, 31-37.
  49. Evans, G.W., & Kim, P. (2007). Childhood poverty and health: Cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychological Science, 18, 953-957.
  50. McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 171–179.
  51. Niskanen, L., Laaksonen, D. E., Nyyssonen, K., Punnonen, K., Valkonen, V., Fuentes, R., Tuomainen, T., Salonen, R., & Salonen, J. (2004). Inflammation, abdominal obesity, and smoking as predictors of hypertension. Hypertension, 44, 859-865.
  52. Amar, J., Fauvel, J., Drouet, L., Ruidavets, J.B., Perret, B., Chamontin, B., Boccalon, H., & Ferrieres, J. (2006). Interleukin 6 is associated with subclinical atherosclerosis: a link with soluble intercellular adhesion molecule 1. Journal of Hypertension, 24, 1083-1086.
  53. Cesari, M., Penninx, B. W., Newman, A. B., Kritchevsky, S. B., Nicklas, B. J., Sutton-Tyrrell K., Rubin, S. M., Ding, J., Simonsick. E. M., Harris, T.B., & Pahor, M. (2003). Inflammatory markers and onset of cardiovascular events: results form the Health ABC study. Circulation, 108, 2317-2322.
  54. Ridker, P. M., Rifai, N., Stampfer, M. J., & Hennekens, C. H. (2000). Plasma concentration of interleukin-6 and the risk of future myocardial infarction among apparently healthy men. Circulation, 101, 1767-1772.
  55. Raison, C. L., Capuron, L., & Miller A. H. (2006). Cytokines sing the blues: Inflammation and the pathogenesis of depression. Trends in Immunology, 27(1), 24-31.
  56. Wellen, K. E., & Hotamisligil, G. S. (2005). Inflammation, stress, and diabetes. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 115(5), 1111-1119.
  57. Coussens, L. M. & Werb, Z. (2002). Inflammation and cancer. Nature, 420, 860-867.
  58. Rakoff-Nahoum. (2006). Why cancer and inflammation? Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 79(3-4), 123-130.
  59. Kiecolt Glaser, Preacher, MacCallum, Atkinson, Malarkey, and Glaser, 2003
  60. Steptoe, A., Hamer, & Chida, Y. (2007). The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating inflammatory factors in humas: A review and meta-analysis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 21, 901-912.
  61. Slavich, G. M., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(33), 14817-14822.
  62. Dickerson, S. S., Gable, S. L., Irwin, M. R., Aziz, N., & Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Social-evaluative threat and proinflammatory cytokine regulation: An experimental laboratory investigation. Psychological Science, 20, 1237-1244.
  63. Miller, G. E., Rohleder, N., Stetler, C., & Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Clinical depression and regulation of the inflammatory response during acute stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 679-687.
  64. Miller, G. E., Rohleder, N., & Cole, S. W. (2009). Chronic interpersonal stress predicts activation of pro- and anti-inflammatory signaling pathways 6 months later. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 57-62.
  65. Taylor, S.E., Lehman, B.J., Kiefe, C.I., & Seeman, T.E. (2006) Relationship of early life stress and psychological functioning of C-reactive protein in the coronary artery risk development in young adults study. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 819-824.
  66. Wilder, R.L. (1995). Neuroendocrine-immune system interactions and autoimmunity. Annual Review of Immunology, 13, 307-338.
  67. Jan, B.U., Coyle, S.M., Macor, M.A., Reddell, M., Calvano, S.E., & Lowry, S.F. (2010). Relationship of basal heart rate variability to in vivo cytokine responses after endotoxin exposure. Shock, 33(4), 363-368.
  68. Marsland, A.L., Gianaros, P.J., Prather, A.A., Jennings, J.R., Neumann, S.A., & Manuck, S.B. (2007). Stimulated production of proinflammatory cytokines covaries inversely with heart rate variability. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(8), 709-716.